![]() |
|
|
|
|
|
Ted Bergstrom, Economics
|
Leda Cosmides, Psychology |
Paul Hernadi, English
|
John Tooby, Anthropology |
Project Summary
|
|
Introduction | |
Intellectual background | |
Emerging interdisciplinary connections | |
Institutional background | |
Present and Future Funding | |
Participants | |
Research projects
|
Collective Action |
Discursive Universals | |
Eductational initiatives
|
Graduate Emphasis |
Collaboration with UCLA | |
Electronic Presence |
Introduction Top
Converging lines of evidence now suggest that the human mind or brain
is not well characterized as a "blank slate." Instead, it appears to contain
a rich and heterogeneous set of functionally specialized cognitive or neural
programs as part of its evolved species-typical design. Researchers from
the natural sciences, humanities, and social sciences propose to use these
findings as the starting point for a series of investigations designed
to kindle novel research and intellectual exchange. Initially, two lines
of inquiry will be used to give focus to the early phases of interdisciplinary
collaboration: (1) What cognitive programs enable human minds to transform
sets of unrelated individuals into coalitions that can act as coordinated
units, solving what economists call the problem of collective action? (2)
How might evolved cognitive and emotional universals help to explain discursive
universals, as well as other widely recurrent elements in the arts? These
investigations will be pursued through open workshops and jointly planned
experiments (and, where possible, through cross-cultural and neuroscience
studies). As part of this initiative, we will create an Evolutionary Behavioral
and Social Science program involving a formal graduate emphasis at UCSB,
a seminar series, workshops, and a joint graduate program with UCLA.
Intellectual background Top
For most of this century, the most widely accepted view within the social sciences, humanities, and natural sciences was that virtually all human mental content derived from individual experience with the physical or social world. That is, adult mental content was acquired or constructed through the operation of general-purpose learning mechanisms that, like the parts of a tape recorder or a camera, impart no content of their own to the outcome: Human nature was viewed as a blank slate plus a capacity to learn culture, and so nothing interesting about culture or social life was believed to arise from human nature itself. Recently, however, a series of findings from evolutionary biology, cognitive science, neuroscience, biological and cultural anthropology, developmental psychology, linguistics, and artificial intelligence have emerged that collectively challenge this view, and that, when integrated, lead to an alternative research framework often called evolutionary psychology.
Evolutionary psychology starts from multiple, converging lines of evidence that the human mind or brain contains, in addition to whatever general learning systems there may be, a large collection of functionally specialized computational devices analogous to expert systems in artificial intelligence or, more loosely, to the specialized application programs people use on their personal computers. Just as personal computers now commonly come factory pre-installed with such specialized programs as a word-processor, spreadsheet, calendar, drawing program, etc., so too the human mind appears to have, as part of its species-typical design, a rich and heterogeneous set of evolved cognitive programs (also called by other terms such as modules or adaptive specializations). These content-specific mental programs may be supplying many of the initial building blocks out of which ideas, institutions, and cultural products are assembled, making the specific attributes of human nature relevant to understanding culture and social interaction.
For example, all normally developing humans from every culture tested appear to have what has been called an intuitive physics or object mechanics (i.e., objects are solid, bounded, permanent, cannot pass through each other or occupy the same space at the same time without deformation, influence each other only by direct contact so that there is no action at a distance, and so on). These cognitive programs seem not to be "learned" in any ordinary sense, but appear through neural maturation on an evolved developmental timetable independent of the specifics of experience or culture, often very early in life (like teeth, such adaptations need not be present at birth). The intuitive physics module, for example, appears already active in infants as early as they can be reliably tested (around two months). Indeed, results suggest that the human brain contains mechanisms that are functionally specialized for cooperation, grammar acquisition, face recognition, threat analysis, food choice, social exchange, snake avoidance, emotional expression recognition and interpretation, aggressive threats, ingroups and coalitions, contagion, hazard avoidance, incest avoidance, learning about the biological world, inferring the contents of others' minds, and scores if not hundreds of other distinct functions.
According to this view, natural selection engineered these cognitive
programs over human evolutionary history to solve the adaptive information-processing
problems regularly encountered by our hunter-gatherer ancestors. The designs
of these programs collectively constitute a precisely characterizable definition
of human nature -- one that can be progressively mapped through careful
cognitive experimentation, neuroscience research, and cross-cultural investigation
through an intrinsically interdisciplinary enterprise. These systems can
be described (1) physically, in terms of the neural structures, chemistry,
and physiological processes involved; (2) computationally, in terms of
their informational properties or meaning: their specialized data formats,
core concepts, specialized operations, and motivational consequences; (3)
behaviorally, in terms of the patterns of behavior, choice, and social
interaction they support; and (4) "humanistically", in terms of how the
activation of such a program is experienced by particular persons, and
what interpretations or meaning systems it guides the individual to subjectively
inhabit or intersubjectively share.
Emerging interdisciplinary connections Top
These discoveries have uncovered close connections between formerly distant fields, and so have deep implications for erasing traditional disciplinary boundaries and prompting new interdisciplinary research collaborations and agendas -- possibilities that UCSB is already taking the lead in exploring. To begin with, evolutionary biology, hunter-gatherer studies, social anthropology, biological anthropology, and cognitive science -- rarely joined together -- are being integrated to allow the joint development of models of the adaptive problems hominids faced and the problem-solving cognitive programs that hominids evolved to solve them. These models are then used to guide the experimental mapping of the structure of these programs, through cognitive experiments, psychological studies, and neuroscience methods. If the evolved cognitive program is an adaptation and has been characterized correctly, it ought to be species-typical, and so anthropological techniques should be able to detect its presence in every culture tested, no matter how different from Western industrial societies in which most psychology is conducted. Moreover, these cognitive programs embody the rules that govern actual economic decision-making, so their mapping will have implications for economic theory, allowing it to be integrated for the first time with accurate models of the psychology of choice. Although existing economic theory tends to work well with large decision-making aggregates, theory and observed behavior sharply diverge in small group settings, especially for group behavior and cooperation -- areas where the evolutionary psychology of hunter-gatherers provides a rapidly developing alternative to rational choice theory through considering the functions and conditions of cooperation in ancestral environments. Reciprocally, economists have developed analytical tools such as game theory that are transforming how evolutionary biologists model of how natural selection operates, creating the new field of evolutionary economics. (Investigators in this project intend to bring together game theory techniques from economics, cognitive experiments, and field experiments among foraging peoples to see if they can develop better models of coalitional behavior than rational choice theory supplies.)
The newest interconnections are with the humanities. An accurate and
detailed model of human nature, whether expressed in terms of a universal
human mental design, an inventory of "innate ideas," a description of the
functional architecture of human emotions, or an outline of universal interpretive
modalities cannot help but be centrally relevant to literature, history,
philosophy, linguistics, and the other disciplines in the humanities. For
example, over the last three years, graduate students in UCSB's English
Dept. (Michelle Scalise, Francis Steen) who are working in the Center (see
below) have led the way, with two doctoral dissertations now underway specifically
applying these new ideas to questions in literature and culture history.
New techniques are also being imported into the study of literature and
history: Francis Steen is conducting experiments designed to see whether
conceptual primitives present in our evolved intuitive physics might explain
certain aspects of Renaissance science, and Michelle Scalise is conducting
experiments on the nature of how cognitive dimensions of memorability shape
differential retention of some elements of folklore. Because the human
mind is richly generative and combinatorial, and capable of constructing
an inexhaustible array of specific mental contents out of these evolved
building blocks, there is no risk that the humanities will be subjected
to some form of scientistic reductionism. Indeed, because what can be experimentally
established is always minute in comparison to the complexity of human experience,
the humanities will continue to have at least as much to offer the sciences
as the reverse (e.g., by pointing out phenomena, anomalies, overlooked
questions, etc.).
Institutional background Top
UCSB has developed what is arguably the largest and most active community of researchers in this new area in the world. To help give institutional form to this cross-departmental strength, UCSB's Office of Research established the Center for Evolutionary Psychology three years ago (Leda Cosmides and John Tooby are the co-directors). Since then, UCSB has developed the first doctoral programs anywhere to offer training in evolutionary psychology (in two new wings in anthropology and psychology). Other departments are increasingly involved, with a new initiative and faculty recruitment effort by the Economics Department in evolutionary economics, and with strong affiliated programs in such areas as evolutionary biology (UCSB has one of the four highest ranked programs in the nation) and hunter-gatherer archaeology.
This project will allow the Center to increasingly facilitate cross-disciplinary
research, providing a series of services to the UCSB community: The Center
brings together research teams across departments, and organizes collaborations
with researchers at other universities both at a faculty level and graduate
student level. In addition to the cognitive science and experimental economics
laboratories located in the psychology and anthropology departments, the
Center maintains a field station among isolated hunter-horticulturalists
in the Ecuadorian Amazon. The purpose of this field station is to provide
the UCSB research community with the opportunity to conduct cross-cultural
experiments (testing the species-typicality of cognitive programs and behavioral
patterns as part of the Human Universals Project) and studies of social
interaction in small, face to face communities ecologically more similar
to the social milieus in which our minds evolved. The Center also maintains
active collaborative relationships with the Center for Neuroscience at
UC Davis and the Neuroimaging Laboratory at University College London,
where hypotheses developed by Center affiliates (faculty and graduate students)
can be tested using neuroscience techniques. Educationally, approximately
14 graduate students (from anthropology, economics, English, and psychology)
are conducting their graduate research through the Center, and the Center
facilitates undergraduate research through assisting interns and senior
thesis research, as well as developing curricular materials. Since its
inception, the Center has offered a weekly seminar oriented towards faculty,
graduate students, and advanced undergraduates, which as of last quarter
developed into the Evolutionary Behavioral and Social Science (EBSS) seminars
sponsored by faculty from the EEMB, anthropology, psychology, and economics
departments. Beyond UCSB, the Center acts as a worldwide clearinghouse
and coordinator for research and education in these emerging areas. Although
there are individual scholars of great distinction in this field at other
universities, there is no comparable institutionalized center or research
community at any other university, and certainly no group that integrates
more than two of these six or eight allied fields. The Center has a ten-member
advisory board drawn both from a subset of participating UCSB faculty and
from researchers at other campuses (e.g., Irven DeVore, Director of the
Peabody Museum & Chair, Dept. of Anthropology, Harvard; Paul Ekman,
Human Interaction Lab, UCSF; Michael Gazzaniga, Director, Program for Cognitive
Neuroscience, Dartmouth, Steven Pinker, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience,
MIT, Roger Shepard, Dept. of Psychology, Stanford).
Present and Future Funding Top
With some contrivances, the Center has been able to foster a significant
amount of interdisciplinary research, despite the fact that it so far has
had no funding of its own. To date, it has been operating on discretionary
funds drawn from an NSF Presidential Young Investigator grant to John Tooby,
a grant that expires this July. To judge by program officer encouragement,
and the intense scholarly interest in the research being produced at the
Center, there are increasing signs that both federal and foundation funding
might be successfully pursued over the next two years, especially if the
Center can maintain or expand its activities.
Participants Top
The four PIs (Cosmides, Hernadi, Tooby, and Bergstrom) are meant to
form an organizational core drawn from the natural sciences, social sciences,
and humanities that will help to coordinate the activities of a far larger
and widening circle of interested researchers. Since the preproposal was
submitted, a meeting was held among interested faculty from at least eight
departments (from the EEMB and psychology in the natural sciences to English
and history in the humanities). At this meeting, the RAD proposal was discussed,
and a decision was taken to attempt to create a formal graduate emphasis
at UCSB in the Evolutionary Behavioral and Social Sciences (EBSS), together
with a set of related institutions, such as a weekly seminar series. In
addition, some of the other parties likely to be interested in the RAD
proposal were approached on a one-on-one basis, as time permitted. Although
individuals who have expressed an interest in participating in this project
in either or both the EBSS components and the specific research components
are too numerous to list, they include Robert Warner, Steve Rothstein,
and Jim Reichman from the EEMB (who have been participating since January);
and H. Porter Abbott and Robert Erickson (English), Wolf Kittler (Chair,
Germanic, Slavic and Semitic Studies), Victoria Vesna (Art), Bert States,
Davies King, and -- as time permits -- Simon Williams (Drama, IHC), who
have been contacted more recently. Although there are a large number of
other interested participants (e.g., Don Zimmerman, Sociology; Napoleon
Chagnon, Don Symons, and Don Brown, from Anthropology) we have mentioned
these researchers individually in order to address two areas of special
concern brought up by the reviewers -- whether members of the life sciences
were involved, and whether we intended to include more participants from
the humanities generally, and from the fine and performing arts specifically.
Activities Top
To give the research component an initial focus and momentum, two lines
of inquiry will be used to organize the early phases of interdisciplinary
collaboration. However, the core group fully anticipates that once the
open workshops and initial project phases establish a pattern of regular
interchange among researchers who have not previously interacted, new lines
of investigation and new research teams will emerge to complement the initial
research directions that are being used to ignite interest. Indeed, our
experience with potential participants is that even short discussions on
these topics lead to the identification of a series of promising ideas
and issues to pursue. The two inaugural research projects are:
1. Collective Action Top
What evolved, species-typical decision-making mechanisms and social
emotions underlie our species' zoologically unusual ability to form shifting
coalitions or groups of often unrelated individuals that act cooperatively
towards common goals (often in opposition to other groups)? Despite intense
selection pressures for winning access to disputed resources such as mates,
members of very few other species appear to be able to solve what economists
identify as the game theoretic or evolutionary obstacles to the emergence
of collective action (free-rider problems, coordination problems, information-limitation
problems, etc.). This reseach thread involves experiments at the psychological
level, aimed at elucidating the computational machinery that guides when
one construes the self as a member of a cooperating group or coalition,
and how one reasons about such situations; studies at the population level,
using the methods of experimental economics to illuminate the online dynamics
of group cooperation when individuals actually interact; game theoretic
modeling of group cooperation (with and without family and kinship elements),
to discover the nature of the selection pressures that emerge when individuals
cooperate in groups larger than two; and field studies of coalitional cooperation
at the CEP field site in Ecuador (e.g., among the Shiwiar and Achuar peoples).
For example, how does perceived membership in coalitions affect the activation
of decision rules that regulate cooperation, defection, and punishment
in dynamic interactions? Since the question of the nature of collective
identities is a prominent feature in much recent discourse in the humanities,
it is hoped that this research theme will benefit from input from participants
from literature, history, and related fields. In addition to the research
itself, the participants envision a series of workshops led by Bergstrom
and other UCSB economists on how to apply game theory and other tools from
economics to evolutionary questions and small group dynamics.
2. Discursive Universals Top
How do evolved cognitive universals explain or participate in discursive universals? In earlier work, Hernadi identified four combinable modes of discourse as possibly universal building blocks of literary works (thematic, lyric, narrative, or dramatic) -- modes that also occur in oral cultures that transmit proverbs, songs, stories, and public rituals from one generation to the next. The extent to which various discursive modes and strategies are in fact universal, and the extent to which their wide-spread occurrence relies on biological conditions of intelligibility rather than semiotic/hermeneutic conditions of communicability will be explored rather than presupposed.
One of the additional topics that the participants wish to explore is the relationship between the evolved structure of specialized memory systems, such as episodic memory, and the structure of discourse and narrative. Memory and neuroscience research has established that humans automatically and nonconsciously organize their experience into bounded units, with the boundaries being established by changes in location, time, interactants, social variables, and other similar factors, even though the underlying "objective" reality is continuous. A related question is the extent to which the memory and representational systems implicated in discourse are specialized to deal with social entities and events (agents and social collectivities) rather than the physical world.
Another anticipated collaboration involves bringing humanists familiar with various cultural media and traditions together with anthropologists, psychologists, and biologists to explore the wide-spread human tendency to respond with incipient or fully-fledged tears or laughter to the "thrilling" or "gratifying" entertainment provided by staged, narrated, and filmed fiction. This brings up the broader question of what an understanding of the growing inventory of known, cross-culturally universal emotional adaptations, such as sexual jealousy, romantic love, or grief may offer to humanists.
This leads to more difficult and perplexing questions concerning (1) the extent to which the human ability to imagine fictive worlds evolved as an aspect of the social task of constructing representations of the unknown intentions of others, or as a way of prompting the imaginative contemplation of alternatives to a person's or culture's primary assumptions about reality; and (2) more broadly, why have humans evolved aesthetic sentiments, capacities, and appetites at all?
We hope to pursue not only the coalitional questions empirically and
experimentally, but also, where the questions can be posed precisely enough
(as with episodic memory or the impact of laughter or grief), the humanistic
questions as well. We have a number of techniques available, including
cognitive experiments (involving, e.g., inference or memory), social psychological
and experimental economics approaches, and the assay of changes in cortisol,
testosterone, and estradiol, through analysis of saliva samples. Where
protocols can be developed, some of these issues could be explored cross-culturally
at the field site, or through cognitive neuroscience techniques.
Educational Initiatives Top
In addition to the research component, one educational component of
this initiative will be to formalize a new graduate emphasis in this area
at UCSB. A second is to pursue a similar formalization of a joint program
with UCLA. To date, over two dozen faculty at UCSB and UCLA have expressed
an interest in forming an interdisciplinary graduate program, perhaps jointly
between UCSB and UCLA, in the Evolutionary Behavioral and Social Sciences
(EBSS). The increasing interest from faculty in the humanities suggests
that this initiative be broadened to reflect their participation and perspective
as well.
Electronic Presence Top
A second educational component will be the development of a web site that contains a hypertext map of the constituent fields and their conceptual interconnections; key references with annotations; on-line tutorials; on-line cognitive experiments; virtual interactive models of cognitive structures -- a specified "virtual brain" containing what is known about these mechanisms. The site will also serve as a mechanism for the researchers involved to make their research easily retrievable by their colleagues in the group (a password-protected area of the site restricted to project participants); and as a way for the members of the research team to make their results publicly available through electronic publishing.
![]() |
|
![]() |